How Stereotypes Shape the Language People Use
As the United States celebrates its the second year with Juneteenth as a federal
holiday, many articles will be written about race relations. But I’d like to broach
one topic that often falls under the radar: stereotypes.
From the first instant our eyes alight on
a television or phone screen, we are
inundated with a curated set of images that (supposedly) depict the world around
us. These images often show people of color through a stereotypical lens, and
these stereotypes bleed into our everyday lives—our workplaces, our social lives,
our politics. As a social psychologist at Yale University, I am figuring out exactly
how stereotypes hold us back, and what we can do about it.
When I was a young Black girl growing up in Prince George’s County,
Maryland,
I loved the movies. Each year, my brothers and I would gleefully wait in line to
get the best seat in the theater for the latest Lord of the Rings, Harry Potter, or
superhero film.
Even then, I was struck by the characters I saw. Few looked like me or my family.
Those that did were one-dimensional, with limited speaking roles, often playing
supporting roles to White characters. They were disproportionately poor and often
criminal.
They were rarely desired, easily disposed of, and never granted the
nuanced and flawed inner worlds granted to White characters.
These stereotypes puzzled me. Prince George’s County, Maryland, is a
majorityBlack county—home to doctors, lawyers, politicians,
and other Black
professionals. The Black characters I saw on television didn’t reflect the rich,
diverse, and joyful lives I saw around me. Why does the media put people of color
into boxes? How do these stereotypes harm us as individuals and a society?
I became a social psychologist to answer these questions. Twenty years later, I
now study stereotypes, determining how they maintain inequality and worm their
way into day-to-day interactions. Across dozens of studies featuring thousands of
participants, I find that stereotypes influence how we relate to others, leaking into
conversations through the very words that people use.
In one test, I focused on White Americans.
White people are subject to
stereotypes, too. They’re labeled as more competent than Black people and
Latina/os, and White people think that other racial groups see them as racist and
entitled. I predicted that White Americans, particularly those who want to connect
across racial divides— White liberals—try to reverse these stereotypes through
the very words that they use.
I focused on those who are more conservative because they tend to have more
positive attitudes toward White Americans and negative attitudes toward their
own racial group.
I found that Black Americans and Latina/os who
were more conservative used
more competent language than their more liberal peers in these mostly-White
settings. (There was no such effect among White politicians,
or when I asked
Black people to talk to other Black people.) These data suggest that people have
a profound desire to reverse negative stereotypes, and this desire shows up in
everyday conversation. Stereotypes force us into rigid boxes, and we try to break
free of them using the most primary tool available to us: our words.
Now an adult, I still love mainstream television and movies—and I am still largely
disappointed by what I see. Most characters are White, the vast majority of spoken
lines go to White characters, and many Black characters are rooted in stereotypes.
(The latest season of Netflix’s hit Stranger Things provides a vivid example.)
Awareness and research can help us understand what stereotypes are and how they
are harmful, but until we enact large-scale, cultural changes that challenge these
stereotypes, we will all continue to be shackled by them.
I asked over two thousand White Americans to introduce themselves to a Black
or White person online. As predicted, White liberals used fewer words related to
competence (like “competitive” or “powerful”) when speaking to a Black person.
This “competence downshift” isn’t limited to a lab. I analyzed over 20 years of
campaign speeches by White Democratic and Republican presidential candidates
and found that White Democrats used fewer words related to competence when
addressing mostly-minority audiences (e.g., NAACP ) versus mostly-White ones
(e.g., American Federation of Teachers). White Republicans didn’t downshift
competence, likely because they’re less interested in getting along with people of
color. Sure enough, White Democrats were more likely to address audiences of
color than Republicans.
For
White liberals, this behavior may backfire. My colleagues and I are now
testing whether White liberals who use less competent language are seen as
patronizing by Black observers. If so, they may reduce, rather than improve, their
chances of crossracial connection by downshifting competence.
Do people of
color also counter stereotypes using language? To find out, I analyzed 250,000
congressional remarks and one million tweets by Black and Latina/o politicians
in Congress and Twitter. I focused on Black Americans and Latina/os because
they tend to be stereotyped as lower in status and power than White Americans.