Welcome to Mr Aslanov’s Lessons QUESTION-TYPE BASED TESTS FunEnglishwithme +99894 6333230
such basic concepts as gravity, solidity and contiguity. In one such experiment, by University of Illinois
psychologist Renee Baillargeon, a hinged wooden panel appeared to pass right through a box. Baillargeon
and M.I.T’s Elizabeth Spelke found that babies as young as 3 1/2 months would reliably look longer at the
impossible event than at the normal one. Their conclusion: babies have enough built-in knowledge to
recognise that something is wrong.
Sirois does not take issue with the way these experiments were conducted. “The methods are correct
and replicable,” he says, “it’s the interpretation that’s the problem.” In a critical review to be published in
the forthcoming issue of the European Journal of Developmental Psychology, he and Jackson pour cold
water over recent experiments that claim to have observed innate or precocious social cognition skills in
infants. His own experiments indicate that a baby’s fascination with physically impossible events merely
reflects a response to stimuli that are novel. Data from the eye tracker and the measurement of the pupils
(which widen in response to arousal or interest) show that impossible events involving familiar objects are
no more interesting than possible events involving novel objects. In other words, when Daniel had seen the
red train come out of the tunnel green a few times, he gets as bored as when it stays the same color. The
mistake of previous research, says Sirois, has been to leap to the conclusion that infants can understand the
concept of impossibility from the mere fact that they are able to perceive some novelty in it. “The real
explanation is boring,” he says.
So how do babies bridge the gap between knowing squat and drawing triangles a task Daniel’s sister
Lois, 2 1/2, is happily tackling as she waits for her brother? “Babies have to learn everything, but as Piaget
was saying, they start with a few primitive reflexes that get things going,” said Sirois. For example,
hardwired in the brain is an instinct that draws a baby’s eyes to a human face. From brain imaging studies
we also know that the brain has some sort of visual buffer that continues to represent objects after they have
been removed a lingering perception rather than conceptual understanding. So when babies encounter novel
or unexpected events, Sirois explains, “there’s a mismatch between the buffer and the information they’re
getting at that moment. And what you do when you’ve got a mismatch is you try to clear the buffer. And
that takes attention.” So learning, says Sirois, is essentially the laborious business of resolving mismatches.
“The thing is, you can do a lot of it with this wet sticky thing called a brain. It’s a fantastic, statistical-
learning machine”. Daniel, exams ended, picks up a plastic tiger and, chewing thoughtfully upon its heat,
smiles as if to agree.