led to the 1933 publication of
Hypnosis and Suggestibility, a classic text. More recently, the eminent experimental psychologist E.R. Hilgard (
8
) of Stanford University
has made major contributions to establishing the scientific credibility of hypnosis with quantitative empirical studies of individual differences in hypnotic ability as well
as a theory of hypnosis as “divided consciousness” (
11
).
Many physicians and psychologists are unaware that in 1955 the British Medical Association cautiously recommended the teaching of hypnosis in medical schools
and its use in clinical practice. In 1958, the American Medical Association made a similar recommendation. In 1960, the American Psychological Association officially
recognized the American Board of Psychological Hypnosis (ABPH) and its authority to examine and certify diplomates with advanced competence in either
experimental or clinical hypnosis. There are now similar national boards identifying advanced competence in medical and dental hypnosis.
Model: Mind-Body Interaction
In 1979, a model of mind-body interaction, in which hypnotic ability was a central component and beliefs were hypothesized to have biologic consequences, was
proposed (
1
,
3
,
12
,
13
and
14
). There is now evidence from cognitive neuroscience that causality operates in both a top-down and a bottom-up sense (
9
). Recent
empiric neuroscience work has been specifying the neuroendocrine and immune links between cognitions, emotions, and biology (
2
). There is growing evidence that
beliefs may have biologic consequences through neuroendocrine and immune mechanisms (
1
,
3
,
12
,
13
and
14
). In fact, in highly hypnotizable persons and under
certain conditions (e.g., during stress, highly supportive environments), beliefs can have potent, specific, and reliable biologic consequences
that range from allergic
reactions, warts, or congenital skin diseases to changes in mammary glands, proneness of the skin to burn or be burnt
, and the inhibition of bleeding (
1
,
12
,
15
).
Converging evidence shows that if threatening beliefs are blocked from consciousness, under some circumstances they may drive somatic symptoms (
1
,
3
,
12
,
14
,
16
,
17
and
18
) like chronic pain, insomnia, and irritable bowel syndrome; these circumstances may include major life changes, times of low social support, or the
individual having a high Marlowe-Crowne score (an operational definition of repression [
19
]).
These physiologic effects in highly hypnotizable subjects can be more specific and rapid than the effects of drugs. For example, one study (
20
) demonstrated that it is
possible under hypnotic control for an individual to demonstrate significant increases in peripheral skin temperature in one hand and significant decreases
concurrently in the other hand. There is no drug that can have such concurrent and specific selective effects on the human body.
Hypnosis and the High-Risk Model of Threat Perception
In highly hypnotizable and motivated persons, hypnosis results in apparently involuntary changes in perception, memory, and mood; these changes can have
profound behavioral and biologic consequences (
1
,
3
,
14
). This perception of changes occurring without effort or involuntarily is the litmus test of true hypnosis (
21
,
22
). At a baseline level, there are large individual differences in people's response to hypnotic suggestions. There appears to be an association between a patient's
measured hypnotic ability and the clinical efficacy of hypnotherapy for several somatic and psychologic symptoms (
5
,
23
,
24
). There is evidence from the high-risk
model of threat perception (HRMTP) (
1
,
3
,
12
,
13
and
14
,
16
,
25
) that high and also low hypnotic abilities, in interaction with conscious or unconscious negative
affect, may be risk factors for several psychophysiologic disorders and some psychological disorders (
1
,
12
,
14
,
25
). “Highs” will develop a mix of psychological (e.g.,
anxiety, depression) and somatic (e.g., pain, sleep)
symptoms, but “lows” will develop mainly somatic symptoms during trauma or stress (
1
,
12
,
14
).
The HRMTP predicts that persons who are high in hypnotic ability will be at risk for stress-related disorders because of their hypersensitivity to the perception of
threat; a superior capacity for operant conditioning; an ability to keep secrets from self (e.g., the ability to block memory and perception, as in posthypnotic amnesia
and surgical hypnoanalgesia); a propensity to surplus pattern recognition (their tendency to see meaning in apparently randomly distributed events); and a tendency
to surplus empathy (their tendency to absorb the emotions of others, with indistinct interpersonal boundaries). The following symptoms have been found to be related
to a patient's measured baseline hypnotic ability: migraine pain intensity (
26
); experimental pain intensity (
27
); facial pain intensity (
28
); intensity of chronic urticaria
(
29
); atopic eczema (
30
); severity of clinical and dental phobias (
31
,
32
); negative moods (
33
,
34
); major depression (
35
); posttraumatic stress disorder (
36
,
37
);
dissociative disorders (
38
,
39
); predisposition to nightmares (
40
); EEG–defined insomnia (
3
); substance abuse and bulimia (
35
); moderate and morbid obesity (
3
,
41
);
and nausea and vomiting during pregnancy (
42
).
This vulnerability of highly hypnotizable persons is probably related to peculiarities in their perception, memory, and mood that amplify negative affect (
14
,
16
). A
subset of patients who have chronic somatic complaints that are unresponsive to standard medical or surgical therapy are high or low in hypnotic ability (
14
). Hypnotic
ability is related, in a dose-response manner, to sympathetic electrodermal reactivity (EDR) during experimentally-induced cognitive stress, and hypnotizability
interacts with experimentally-induced cognitive stress (
16
) to drive up EDR in chronic pain patients. High hypnotizability also appears related to alterations in immune
function (
43
). There is evidence that low hypnotic ability during stress may be a risk factor for somatic symptoms associated with pathophysiology (
14
,
44
,
45
and
46
).
These symptoms include chronic pain, morbid obesity, response to cardiac surgery, chest pain, and insomnia. It appears that these low hypnotizable patients,
because of their rigidly skeptical cognitive style and denial of psychological causation, have limited
psychological coping skills, are hyposensitive to psychosocial
threat, and delay seeking diagnostic investigation of their symptoms (
1
,
14
,
41
). High hypnotic ability is related to sympathetic hyperactivity, and low hypnotic ability is
related to dysregulation of the parasympathetic nervous system during stress or trauma (
19
,
41
).
PRINCIPAL CONCEPTS
Despite prior controversy, sophisticated signal processing techniques have shown that, under baseline conditions and during and after hypnotic induction, there are
systematic EEG differences between carefully selected high and low hypnotizable subjects (
47
). It is worth noting that these electrophysiologic differences exist in the
frontal and temporal cortex, and that they distinguish between high and low hypnotizable subjects at baseline before any hypnotic induction. These converging data
are consistent with the hypothesis that high and low hypnotizables have different cognitive styles and process information very differently both outside of and within
hypnosis (
1
,
3
,
12
,
14
), and that using biofeedback to increase theta EEG waves may at least temporarily increase hypnotic ability (
48
,
49
). Recent work suggests that
measuring baseline frontal and temporal theta waves may provide an electrophysiologic test of hypnotic ability.
Hypnotizability and hypnosis have also been shown to be related to the ability to verbally alter a variety of basic physiologic, electrophysiologic, and conditioning
phenomena (operant and respondent) during hypnosis. For example, Klein and Spiegel (
50
) and Whorwell et al. (
51
) showed that hypnotizability or hypnosis could
stimulate and inhibit both gastric acid secretion and a colonic motility index. In a controlled study, Ruzyla-Smith et al. (
43
) showed that hypnotizability was related to
alterations in B cells and helper T cells of the immune system. Black (
52
) and Zachariae et al. (
53
) showed that high hypnotic ability was related to the inhibition of the
Mantoux
reaction to tuberculin, and that during hypnosis the Mantoux reaction could be selectively increased in one arm and decreased in the other. For persons who
are highly hypnotizable, hypnotic analgesia is as effective as morphine and more effective than acupuncture (
54
,
55
). Also, naloxone does not block the mechanism of
hypnotic analgesia (
56
,
57
). It has been found that hypnotically suggested visual hallucinations alter cortical EEG event-related potentials and not simply verbal
reports (
58
,
59
and
60
). High hypnotic ability can increase the rate of acquisition of learning in both operant (
61
,
62
,
63
,
64
and
65
) and Pavlovian conditioning (
2
)
situations. Hence, it is not surprising to find that high hypnotic subjects respond more rapidly to various types of short-term psychotherapy (
66
,
67
) and appear to learn
both adaptive and maladaptive responses rapidly and unconsciously (
1
,
3
,
12
,
13
,
19
).
PROVIDER-PATIENT INTERACTIONS
Hypnotizability
Hypnotherapy is the use of hypnosis for treatment and is usually added to some established form of psychosocial diagnosis and therapy (
65
), such as psychodynamic
psychotherapy (
7
), behavior therapy (
65
,
85
), or biofeedback therapy (
65
). It appears that a hypnotic induction can potentiate verbal instructions in any social
influence situation (
65
,
86
,
87
and
88
). After a hypnotic induction, a variety of psychological techniques, such as age regression (psychodynamic therapy), guided
imagery, systematic desensitization (behavior therapy), or the delayed or immediate feedback of biologic information (biofeedback), may reduce specific clinical
symptoms (
89
). The clinical efficacy of blocking or recovering painful, fantasized, or real memories or altering experimental pain perception is related to hypnotic
ability (
11
). The hypnotic induction ritual increases, at least moderately, the following factors:
1. Suggestibility
2. Imagery and fantasy ability
3. Access to primitive modes of information processing
4. Access to early childhood memories and fantasies
5. Tolerance for logical incongruities (e.g., “trance logic,” which is the acceptance of the suggestion that a person is in two different places at the same time)
6. Alteration or inhibition of cognition;
selective amnesia
7. Creativity
8. Alterations in the perception of sensory events and muscular response
Empirical evidence exists showing that most of these eight factors can be increased to some degree by a hypnotic induction ritual particularly for persons who have
high hypnotic ability (
90
,
91
). Hypnotic ability, like absorption, appears to be a normally distributed stable trait (
8
,
92
), with a .71 test-retest correlation after 25 years.
It also appears to be partly genetically based (
93
,
94
).
Hypnotizability associated with a “nonvolitional” response to hypnotic suggestions (
22
) is not compliance, conformity, gullibility, or social desirability (
8
,
14
). Nor is
hypnotizability correlated significantly with any other known personality variable measured on any standard personality tests, such as the Minnesota Multi-phasic
Personality Inventory (MMPI), California Personality Inventory (CPI), Neuroticism Extroversion Openness (NEO) Personality Inventory, Eysenck Personality Inventory,
or the Meyers Briggs Inventory (
8
,
14
). Hypnotic ability is an essential, but not a sufficient, condition to demonstrate the aforementioned eight alterations in
perception, memory, and mood. The highly hypnotizable person must also be motivated to participate in the hypnotic induction and to respond to the verbal
instructions in hypnosis (
22
,
65
).
Tests of Hypnotizability
There are a number of tests of hypnotizability, each of which has strengths and weaknesses. Hypnotic depth measurements made by clinicians 150 years ago are
generally in agreement with psychometrically more reliable and valid procedures developed in the last 50 years. For example, the estimates of the percent of people
in the general population with high hypnotic ability and those with low hypnotic ability are similar (
68
,
69
).
S
TANFORD
H
YPNOTIC
S
USCEPTIBILITY
S
CALE
, F
ORM
C
Currently, the gold standard in the measurement of hypnotic ability is the Stanford
Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C (
69
,
70
). However, the Stanford Form C has
many difficult cognitive items, and its distribution of scores is not normal, but it taps a broader range of hypnotic abilities than do other tests. Its use in routine clinical
practice is not practical for several reasons. First, this test can be given to only one patient at a time, and it takes nearly one hour to administer. Second, its use in a
clinical practice requires a skilled clinician, and it can generate a high rate (29–31%) of negative side effects (e.g., temporary headache or disorientation, nausea),
even with otherwise healthy college students (
71
,
72
).
H
ARVARD
G
ROUP
S
CALE OF
H
YPNOTIC
S
USCEPTIBILITY
, F
ORM
A
For clinical research, there are several reasons to use the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A (
73
) (HGSHS:A) with congruent subjective
validation (
21
). The Harvard scale was found to correlate .68 (p <.0001) with the gold standard in a recent study (
70
), and other studies have also found correlations
as high as .84 (
74
). It was found that the HGSHS:A correctly classified more than 80% of highly hypnotizable persons (
74
). Group testing with the addition of
subjective validation criteria (
21
) permits a skilled technician working under the supervision of a clinician to test 5 to 10 patients in 1 hour with a minimal rate (less
than 3%) of negative side effects (e.g., temporary headaches and disorientation, nausea). Using the Harvard scale, Crawford et al. reported a 5% rate of similar
negative side effects in normal college students (
71
).
After being tested on the Harvard scale, the bulk of patients (85%) in a behavioral medicine clinic reported temporarily increased comfort and relaxation.
Approximately 5% of these patients even reported immediate temporary relief of their presenting clinical symptom or symptoms. This nonspecific therapy response is
brief, but it grabs the patient's attention and improves general compliance. The Harvard scale correlates (r = .74) with the Stanford, Form C (
75
), which has been used
extensively in large-scale longitudinal and cross-sectional studies of the stability and genetics of hypnotic ability. The Harvard scale has norms on large cross-cultural
non-patient samples (
69
) and even on patients (
14
,
35
,
76
,
77
). Also, this
scale has high reliability, and its validity can be increased by using congruent, subjective
scoring procedures (
21
,
78
). Using a technician to administer the Harvard scale avoids contaminating the clinician–patient relationship with “failed” test suggestions.
Failed test suggestions are hypnotic suggestions the patient could not experience. In clinical research today, the Harvard scale, along with subjective scoring, has
many merits as a first test of hypnotizability. The patient's score can tell what disorders and types of symptoms (somatic or psychological) this patient is at risk for and
what the mean rate of therapeutic response will be to learning-based treatments (e.g., biofeedback, cognitive behavior therapy).
H
YPNOTIC
I
NDUCTION
P
ROFILE
For purposes of rapid routine clinical screening, the Hypnotic Induction Profile (HIP) (
79
) has many merits. It is a brief test (10 minutes) that is minimally challenging to
the patient and presents hypnosis as a subtle perceptual alteration involving a capacity for attention, responsiveness, and concentration that is inherent in the person
and can be tapped by the examiner (
79
). The HIP has two components: the eye roll sign and the induction (IND). The eye roll sign appears to correlate .34 (p < .001)
with the Stanford, Scale C (
69
). The eye roll is a biologic marker of hypnotic potential but not necessarily of typical hypnotic performance. The IND score on the HIP
correlated at .63 with the Stanford, Form C, and when depth ratings from the two scales were compared, the correlation was .78 (
80
). The aforementioned
considerations suggest that the HIP may be a useful, brief measure of hypnotic ability in clinical demonstrations and in routine clinical practice. Its limited acceptance
by the research community is a constraint on its use in clinical research.
A
BSORPTION
T
EST
All the previously mentioned tests of hypnotizability involve a standardized hypnotic induction and one or more behavioral or verbal report measures of response to
standardized hypnotic suggestions. The absorption test involves neither a hypnotic induction nor an actual measure of response to standardized suggestions (
81
).
The absorption test is a short (10 minute) paper-and-pencil test that correlates moderately with the Harvard and Stanford tests. It inquires about the frequency of
naturally occurring hypnotic-like experiences (e.g., the ability to use fantasy and to ignore distractions in everyday life). On some theoretical
and empirical grounds, it
can be considered a good measure of true hypnotic ability and one of the most difficult cognitive tests of hypnotic capacity (
22
).
Absorption is a personality trait that is normally distributed, is stable (30-day retest, r = .91), and appears—like hypnotic ability—to be partly genetically based in
monozygotic twins reared apart (
82
) and independent of context effects (
83
). Like hypnotic ability (
1
,
12
,
14
,
16
,
25
), absorption has also been shown to be a risk
factor for several stress-related disorders, such as nonorganic chest pain (
45
) and somatic complaints in family medicine, as well as morbid obesity (
41
), nightmares
(
40
), and anticipatory nausea and vomiting secondary to chemotherapy (
84
).
Absorption appears to provide a nonintrusive measure of hypnotic ability, particularly at the high (above 75%) and low (below 25%) ends of the scale. This test can be
given to a patient before a clinical session to obtain a primitive estimate of the patient's hypnotic ability. I suspect that absorption will predict, for example, response to
several clinical interventions like acupuncture, guided imagery, noncontact therapeutic touch, herbal therapy, massage, and so forth (
1
).
Dostları ilə paylaş: