patterns which are amenable to sorting and classification, and that the sequence is circumscribed by
unspoken rules and regulations. These regulations remain latent as long as the amities or hostilities
proceed according to Hoyle, but they become manifest if an illegal move is made, giving rise to a
symbolic, verbal or legal cry of "Foul!" Such sequences, which in contrast to pastimes are based
more on individual than on social programming, may be called games. Family
life and married life,
as well as life in organizations of various kinds, may year after year be based on variations of the
same game.
To say that the bulk of social activity consists of playing games does not necessarily mean that it is
mostly "fun" or that the parties are not seriously engaged in the relationship. On the one hand,
"playing" football and other athletic "games" may not be fun at all, and the players may be
intensely grim; and such games share with gambling and other forms of "play" the potentiality for
being very serious indeed, sometimes fatal. On the other hand, some authors, for instance Huizinga,
9 include under "play" such serious things as cannibal feasts. Hence calling such tragic behavior as
suicide, alcohol and drug addiction, criminality or schizophrenia "playing games" is not
irresponsible, facetious or barbaric. The essential characteristic of human play is not that the
emotions are spurious, but that they are regulated. This is revealed when sanctions are imposed on
an illegitimate emotional display.
Play may be grimly serious, or even fatally serious, but the social
sanctions are serious only if the rules are broken.
Pastimes and games are substitutes for the real living of real intimacy. Because of this they may be
regarded as preliminary engagements rather than as unions, which is why they are characterized as
poignant forms of play. Intimacy begins when individual (usually instinctual) programming
becomes more intense, and both social patterning and ulterior restrictions and motives begin to give
way. It is the only completely satisfying answer to stimulus-hunger, recognition-hunger and
structure-hunger. Its prototype is the act of loving impregnation.
Structure-hunger has the same survival value as stimulus-hunger. Stimulus-hunger and recognition-
hunger express the need to avoid sensory and emotional starvation, both of which lead to biological
deterioration. Structure-hunger expresses the need to avoid boredom, and Kierkegaard10 has
pointed out the evils which result from unstructured time. If it persists for any length of time,
boredom becomes synonymous with emotional starvation and can have the same consequences.
The solitary individual can structure time in two ways: activity and fantasy.
An individual can
remain solitary even in the presence of others, as every schoolteacher knows. When one is a
member of a social aggregation of two or more people, there are several options for structuring
time. In order of complexity, these are: (1) Rituals (2) Pastimes (3) Games (4) Intimacy and (5)
Activity, which may form a matrix for any of the others. The goal of each member of the
aggregation is to obtain as many satisfactions as possible from his transactions with other members.
The more accessible he is, the more satisfactions he can obtain. Most of the programming of his
social operations is automatic. Since some of the "satisfactions" obtained under this programming,
such as self-destructive ones, are difficult to recognize in the usual sense of the word
"satisfactions," it would be better to substitute some more non-committal terra, such as "gains" or
"advantages."
The advantages of social contact revolve around somatic and psychic equilibrium. They are related
to the following factors: (1) the relief of tension (2) the avoidance of noxious situations (3) the
procurement of stroking and (4) the maintenance of an established equilibrium. All these items
have been investigated and discussed in great detail by physiologists, psychologists, and
psychoanalysts. Translated into
terms of social psychiatry, they may be stated as (1) the primary
internal advantages (2) the primary external advantages (3) the secondary advantages and (4) the
existential advantages. The first three parallel the "gains from illness" described by Freud: the
internal paranosic gain, the external paranosic gain, and the eplnosic gain, respectively.11
Experience has shown that it is more useful and enlightening to investigate social transactions from
the point of view of the advantages gained than to treat them as defensive operations. In the first
place, the best defense is to engage in no transactions at all; in the second place, the concept of
6
"defenses" covers only part of the first two classes of advantages, and the rest of them, together
with the third and fourth classes, are lost to this point of view.
The most gratifying forms of social contact, whether or not they are embedded in a matrix of
activity, are games and intimacy.
Prolonged intimacy is rare, and even then it is primarily a private
matter; significant social intercourse most commonly takes the form of games, and that is the
subject which principally concerns us here. For further information about rime-structuring, the
author's book on group dynamics should be consulted.
REFERENCES
1. Berne, E. Transnational Analysis in Psychotherapy, Grove Press, Inc., New York, 1961.
2. Spitz, R. "Hospitalism: Genesis of Psychiatric Conditions in Early Childhood." Psychoanalytic
Study of the Child. 1: 53-74, 1945.
3. Belbenoit, Rene. Dry Guillotine. E. P. Dutton & Company, New York, 1938.
4. Seaton, G. J. Isle of the Damned. Popular Library, New York, 1952.
5. Kinkead, E. In Every War But One. W. W. Norton & Company, New York, 1959.
6. French, J. D. "The Reticular Formation." Scientific American. 196: 54-60, May, 1957.
7. The "colloquialisms" used are those evolved in the course of time at the San Francisco Social
Psychiatry Seminars.
8. Levine, S. 'Stimulation in Infancy." Scientific American. 202: 80-86, May, 1960.
————. "Infantile Experience and Resistance to Physiological Stress." Science. 126: 405,
August 30, 1957.
9. Huizinga, J.
Homo Ludens, Beacon Press, Boston, 1955.
10. Kierkegaard, S. A Kierkegaard Anthology, ed. R. Bretall. Princeton University Press, Princeton,
1947, pp. 22 ff.
11. Freud, S. "General Remarks on Hysterical Attacks." Collected Papers, Hogarth Press, London,
1933, II, p. 102.
————. "Analysis of a Case of Hysteria." Ibid. Ill, p. 54.
12. Berne, E. The Structure and Dynamics of Organizations and Groups. J. B. Lippincott Company,
Philadelphia and Montreal, 1963. (See especially Chapters 11 and 120
7