Figure 3.9.
Long-term potentiation. (Bliss and Lømo 1993. Reprinted by permis
sion of the Physiological Society.)
tion. It turns out that glutamate also attaches to NMDA gates. These gates are
initially blocked by Mg
2+
ions, but as the membrane depolarizes through con
tinued opening of non-NMDA receptor gates, the Mg
2+
ions are dislodged.
Once opened, the larger NMDA receptors admit doubly charged calcium (Ca
2+
)
into the cell interior. There the calcium also interacts with calmodulin and CAM
kinase II, which phosphorylates the non-NMDA glutamate receptors, causing
them to be more readily activated in the future. This process is believed to
be the basis of the physiological process of long-term potentiation and of the
psychological phenomenon of LTM (Grossberg 1968; Lynch 1986; McGlade-
McCulloh et al. 1993).
GABA, on the other hand, is inhibitory. It is released by cortical interneu
rons like basket cells and chandelier cells (figure 3.10). Whereas glutamate
opens gates for positive ions, GABA opens channels for the entry of negative
ions, principally Cl
–
, into the cell. This influx of chloride ions hyperpolarizes the
postsynaptic membrane, from –70 mV to perhaps –80 mV, thereby making it
more difficult for excitatory neurotransmitters like glutamate to depolarize the
membrane and initiate spiking. This hyperpolarization of cells does not have
to match impedance in order to propagate a spike, so inhibitory cell synapses
do not exhibit spines, and they are more commonly found on cell bodies than
in dendritic arbors.
For example, GABAergic chandelier cells synapse preferentially on the
initial axon segments of neocortical pyramidal cells. When stained as in fig
ure 3.10, the terminal knobs of their axon collaterals look like so many candle
sticks, hence their name. The axon terminals of basket cells, on the other
hand, form a “basket” of synapses on the bodies of neighboring excitatory
cells (see figure 4.8).
THE COMMUNICATING CELL
•
49
Figure 3.10.
A chandelier cell. (A camera lucida drawing from Peters and Jones 1984,
365. The bar equals 25
µm. Reprinted by permission of Plenum Publishing Corp.)
By some reports, inhibitory neurons compose less than one-fifth of the total
number of CNS neurons, but Crick and Asanuma (1986) observe that even if
inhibitory cells are outnumbered, they may exercise a disproportionate “veto”
power over excitatory signals by exerting their inhibitory influence on prime
real estate like cell bodies and initial axon segments.
As I have noted, numerous other neurotransmitters are present in the CNS
in lesser quantities. The most important among these are acetylcholine, norad-
renaline, and the G-protein neurotransmitters serotonin and dopamine.
Acetylcholine is the neurotransmitter with which motor neurons cause
muscles to contract and is apparently the principal neurotransmitter of the
parasympathetic (smooth-muscle) nervous system. Acetylcholinergic fibers are
also found widely distributed in neocortex and the midbrain. These seem to
mostly arise from the reticular formation of the brain stem, part of the system
for perceiving pain. Because it is present and easily studied at the neuromus
cular junction, acetylcholine was the first neurotransmitter to be identified.
For his discovery of the neurotransmitter role of acetylcholine, Loewi received
the 1936 Nobel Prize.
Serotonin has been traditionally classed as an inhibitory neurotransmit
ter, although it has recently been found to also exert an excitatory effect upon
cerebral cells (Aghajanian and Marek 1997). Since serotonin levels are selec
tively elevated by antidepressives like fluoxetine (Prozac), serotonin has become
famous for its role in controlling various mood disorders.
Noradrenaline is another widespread neurotransmitter. It is identical to
what elsewhere in the body is called the “hormone” adrenaline, but adrenaline
does not cross the blood-brain barrier. The brain must manufacture its own
supply of adrenaline, which it does from dopamine. In the brain, this adrena
line is called noradrenaline or norepinephrine. In the hippocampus of the brain,
and presumably also in neocortex, which develops from the hippocampus,
50 •
HOW THE BRAIN EVOLVED LANGUAGE
noradrenaline has a complex effect. It inhibits neuronal response to brief
stimuli but increases neuronal response to prolonged stimuli.
Figure 3.11 illustrates this differential effect of noradrenaline on inputs
to a hippocampal pyramidal neuron. In the first case shown in figure 3.11A, a
small, ramped input (below) elicits a spike response (above). By contrast, a
longer, more intense “pulse” input elicits a burst of seven spikes in response to
the pulse onset. However, under the influence of noradrenaline, the ramped
input elicits no response, while the pulse elicits fourteen spikes throughout
the duration of the pulse. That is, noradrenaline inhibits small inputs but makes
the neuron hyperexcitable and responsive to the more intense pulse stimulus.
Madison and Nicoll (1986) observe that this effectively enhances the signal-
to-noise ratio of the neuron’s response. I will elaborate on such effects later.
Dopamine is a CNS neurotransmitter which is chemically transformed into
noradrenaline (both are catecholaminergic). Dopamine deficiency has been
found to be a symptom of Parkinson’s disease. Treatment with L-dopa, a dopam
ine precursor, with subsequent increases in dopamine levels in the subcortical
Figure 3.11.
Effect of noradrenaline on evoked postsynaptic potentials. (Madison
and Nicoll 1986. Reprinted by permission of the Physiological Society.)
THE COMMUNICATING CELL
•
51
basal ganglia, described in the following chapter, has ameliorated the symp
toms of Parkinson’s disease for many patients.
Reverse Messengers
At the same time that neurotransmitter released from the presynaptic knob
acts upon the postsynaptic cell, retrograde neurotransmitters such as nerve growth
factor (NGF; Thoenen 1995) and nitric oxide (Kandel and Hawkins 1992; Snyder
and Bredt 1992) are released from the postsynaptic cell and act upon the pr
esynaptic cell, opening Ca
2+
gates and otherwise facilitating the metabolism of
the presynaptic knob. A kind of intercellular free-trade agreement is set up,
and the economies of both the presynaptic cell and the postsynaptic cell begin
to grow (Skrede and Malthe-Sorenssen 1981; Errington and Bliss 1982).
5
Thus, the cell membrane, life’s first defense in the primordial soup, evolved
to allow cooperative commerce among friendly neighbors. There are many
examples of symbiosis in nature, but the cooperative exchange of ions between
two protoneurons was certainly one of its first and most significant occurrences.
Once two neurons could communicate between themselves, it was relatively
easy for 200 or 2,000 to organize themselves in patterns of self-similarity. It took
something like a billion years for life to progress to the two-celled brain. In
half that time, the two-celled brain evolved a million species, including Homo
loquens with his ten-trillion-celled brain, to which we now turn in chapter 4.
52
•
HOW THE BRAIN EVOLVED LANGUAGE
•
F
O
U
R
•
The Society of Brain
In time, the primitive brains that had developed by the end of chapter 2 sur
rounded themselves with yet another self-similar membrane. Called the noto-
chord, this cartilaginous tube houses the primitive central nervous system of the
phylum Chordata. In ontogeny as well as phylogeny, this notochord further
develops into a hard, tubular backbone, so the members of Chordata have come
to be popularly known as “vertebrates.” The long, hollow notochord/backbone
with its bulbous skull at one end is self-similar to the long, hollow axon and its
cell body, and it serves the self-same purpose of communication in these larger,
more complex organisms. In time, the four-celled model brain of chapter 2
evolved into the trillion-celled human brain.
Elephants and whales have larger brains than humans, and they are prob
ably more intelligent, too. At least they have never engaged in such colossal
stupidity as World War I. But if we must find a way to assert humankind’s sup
posedly superior intellect, we can observe that elephants and whales seem to
devote a good portion of their prodigious brains simply to moving their prodi
gious bulks, and we can note the oft-repeated fact that per kilogram of body,
Homo loquens has the largest brain in the world. But even this measure does
little to support our vanity. Among mammals, birds, and reptiles, there is only
a 5% variation in the ratio of brain size to body size (Martin 1982). The proper
question therefore seems to be, not how big an animal’s brain is or how “intel
ligent” the animal is, but what it does with the brain it has.
Subcerebral Brain Structures
If we peer beneath the surrounding cerebrum, we see the older structures which
the cerebrum has overgrown (figure 4.1). These subcerebral (or “subcortical”)
1
structures are usually not considered specialized for cognition or language, but
52
THE SOCIETY OF BRAIN
•
53
Figure 4.1.
Medial section of the human brain. (CC) Corpus callossum; (CG)
cingulate gyrus; (T) Thalamus; (Cblm) cerebellum; (P) pons; (Med) medulla; (Hc)
hippocampus; (Hy) hypothalamus. (After Montemurro and Bruni 1988. Reprinted
by permission of Oxford University Press.)
they are nonetheless indispensable to higher cortical functioning, so at least a
quick survey is in order.
At the base of the subcerebral brain structures is the brain stem. Like the
primitive notochord, it can be viewed as the source of all further brain devel
opment. The lower part of the brain stem, the medulla (Med in figure 4.1)
conducts afferent (ascending) signals from the periphery to the brain and effer-
ent (descending) signals from the brain to the periphery. These peripheral
connections can be either relatively local (e.g., to the eyes, ears, and mouth)
or quite distant (e.g., to the limbs via the neuromotor highway of axons called
the pyramidal tract).
2
Several diffuse catecholaminergic neurotransmitter systems arise from the
medulla that have important connections to the limbic system and the hypo
thalamus. These include important noradrenaline (norepinephrine) systems
arising from the locus coeruleus, and dopamine systems arising from the substantia
nigra. A serotonergic system arises from the raphe nuclei of the medulla, also
with diffuse connections in the limbic system and cerebrum. These systems
modulate behavior, but in globally autonomic and emotional ways rather than
through cognition.
Rostral to (i.e., above, toward the head of) the medulla is the pons (P in fig
ure 4.1). The pons relays ascending and descending motor signals and is the
primary relay site for signals to and from the cerebellum. Above the pons is the
midbrain, a short section of the brain stem containing the superior and inferior
colliculi. The superior colliculus is a major relay point in the visual system, while
the inferior colliculus is a major relay point along auditory pathways.
54 •
HOW THE BRAIN EVOLVED LANGUAGE
Rostral to the midbrain is the diencephalon. We divide the diencephalon into
three parts: the thalamus, the basal ganglia, and the limbic system. The thalamus (T
in figure 4.1) receives afferent (ascending, incoming) sensory signals and relays
them to the cerebrum. Within the thalamus, the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN)
is an important relay point for visual signals, while the medial geniculate nucleus
(MGN) is the thalamic relay point along auditory pathways. Most afferent sen
sory circuits rise through the dorsal thalamus into the cerebrum. The dorsal
thalamus is enveloped by a sheet of inhibitory neurons. This inhibitory envelope
is called the reticular nucleus of the thalamus (RNT, RTN), even though it is not
nuclear in shape. It should be distinguished from the reticular formation of the brain
stem, which is involved in the more primitive sensation of pain. The RNT and
dorsal thalamus together (sometimes called paleocortex) may be seen as the phy
logenetic precursor of cerebrum. Both paleocortex and cerebrum exhibit on-center
off-surround circuitry, a neuroarchitectural design that will become very impor
tant in subsequent chapters.
The basal ganglia comprise a group of structures including the caudate
nucleus, the putamen, and the globus pallidus. Not visible in figure 4.1, these struc
tures lie behind the plane of the medial section, lateral (alongside) and ante
rior to (to the front of) the thalamus. They have occasionally been linked to
language behavior (Metter et al. 1983; Ullman et al. 1997), but we will view this
link largely in terms of their better-documented involvement with posture and
gross motor control. The basal ganglia are particularly influenced by dopami
nergic signals arising just below in the substantia nigra of the midbrain, and dete
rioration of this dopaminergic pathway is an immediate cause of Parkinson’s
disease.
The Limbic System
Ventral to (beneath) and surrounding the thalamus are the various structures
of the limbic system. The hypothalamus (Hy in figure 4.1) is the seat of physiologi
cal drives (hunger, sex, fear, aggression). Often classified as a gland, it secretes
many hormones, such as vasopressin and oxytocin, and exerts direct influences
on the autonomic nervous system, which in turn controls such functions as heart
rate and breathing. The hypothalamus also directly affects the brain’s pituitary
gland, which secretes a wide range of other hormones. As noted in chapter 3,
these hormones are sometimes classed as neurotransmitters, but since they are
diffusely circulated with effects beyond the nervous system, I prefer to class
them simply as hormones, reserving the term “neurotransmitter” for chemi
cals with more cognitive synaptic effects.
Lateral to the hypothalamus is the amygdala. The amygdala sends pro
jections directly to the caudate nucleus and is sometimes therefore counted
among the basal ganglia. The amygdala is quite directly connected to the
frontal and the motor cortex and has been implicated in the modulation of
emotions.
THE SOCIETY OF BRAIN
•
55
The hippocampus (Hc in figure 4.1) is the amygdala’s sensory counterpart.
In 1966, Milner described a patient, HM, who had suffered from severe tempo
ral lobe epilepsy (see also Milner et al. 1968). In a standard effort to control the
epilepsy, surgeons removed large areas of temporal cortex. In HM’s case, they
also removed substantial portions of the hippocampus. HM’s epilepsy was brought
under control, but a new problem was created. HM developed anterograde amne-
sia: he “forgot the future.” HM was unable to form new memories. He was un
able to learn. He remembered well his wife, friends, and family and his old
neighborhoods and haunts. He could not, however, remember people he met
since his surgery. Each day, he would meet with his doctor and say, “Have we met?”
Milner’s report caused an instant sensation in the neuroscience commu
nity and inspired numerous studies of the hippocampus. The most popular
theoretical explanation of hippocampal function has been a consensus “buffer”
model (see the four-volume series edited by Isaacson and Pribram for the evo
lution of this model). In this consensus model, the hippocampus is viewed as
“working memory,” rather like a computer coprocessor or RAM cache which
performs operations on sensory input or briefly stores data before it is trans
ferred to long-term storage in “declarative memory.” This model has the at
traction of computational metaphor, but it accords a large cognitive role to a
small structure in a brain region otherwise not found to be particularly “cogni
tive.” It is also unable to explain many facts. For example, HM was able to de
velop long-term memory for certain unemotional forms of knowledge like the
solution to the Tower of Hanoi problem.
3
More plausible from our perspec
tive is Gray’s theory of the hippocampus as a “comparator” (Gray 1975, 1982).
Gray’s theory also appeared in the Isaacson and Pribram series (Gray and
Rawlins 1986), but it represented a minority opinion and has been slower to
gain popularity.
Situated posterior to the amygdala and lateral to the hypothalamus, the
hippocampus is also widely connected to the cerebrum and the cingulate gyrus
(sometimes called the “limbic lobe”; (CG in figure 4.1) via the cingulum. The
cingulum is a massive bundle of axons that originate in the parahippocampal
lobe of the temporal lobe and arch up and around the diencephalon, behind
the cingulate gyrus in figure 4.1. (In figure 4.10, the left cingulate gyrus has
been dissected, exposing the cingulum.) Indeed, the hippocampus is so mini
mally differentiated from cerebral cortex that it is sometimes called the “hip
pocampal lobe” of cerebrum. But the hippocampus is also connected to the
mammilary body of the hypothalamus via the fornix. In Gray’s theory, the limbic
system—and the hippocampus in particular—can be seen as moderating be
tween the cognitive information of the cerebrum and the physiological and
emotional drives of the more primitive brain.
We will return to this issue several times in subsequent chapters. For the
present, we simply observe how the tears and tantrums of any two-year-old
demonstrate that learning can be a very emotional business, and that the case
of HM suggests that learning can fail if it is disconnected from primitive sur
vival instincts and drives.
56 •
HOW THE BRAIN EVOLVED LANGUAGE
The Cerebellum
Dorsal to the pons is the cerebellum (Cblm in figure 4.1). The cerebellum, like
the cerebrum, is composed of a rind, or cortex, wrapped around several deep
nuclei (CN in figure 4.2) and divided into two hemispheres. The cerebellum
is especially notable for its control and coordination of fine motor behaviors
like knitting, playing a musical instrument, or speaking a language. This con
trol does not, however, include the planning or initiation of behavior, so as
we shall see, the cerebellum’s role is considerably less “cognitive” than the
cerebrum’s.
The distinctive cerebellar architecture (figure 4.2) is characterized by large
output cells, the Purkinje cells (PC in figure 4.2), which send signals to cerebel
lar subcortical nuclei (CN). Purkinje cells are innervated by climbing fibers (CF),
nonspecific inputs that arise from the inferior olive of the medulla, and by long
by parallel fibers (PF) arising from granule cells (GC), which are the cerebrum’s
principal input of sensory and motor information. Granule cells are innervated
by afferent mossy fibers (MF).
The Purkinje cells are embedded in the regular grid of the cerebellum’s
parallel fibers, forming a matrixlike architecture which is schematized in fig
ure 4.3. The fact that this architecture can be efficiently modeled by using well-
known shortcuts like matrix algebra made it easy to quickly program complex,
putatively cognitive, computational neural networks. But unlike cerebral pyra-
Figure 4.2.
Cerebellar cortex. (Fox 1962. Reprinted by permission of Appleton and
Lange.)
THE SOCIETY OF BRAIN
•
57
Figure 4.3.
Schematic of cerebellar cortex. (Loritz 1991. Reprinted by permission
of Oxford University Press.)
midal cells, cerebellar Purkinje cells are inhibitory. As we shall see, the cerebel
lum therefore does not have the on-center off-surround architecture of cere
bral cortex. As one result, the cerebellum is “swept clean” of residual neural
activity within 0.1 ms of input (Eccles 1977). This is all well and good for the
processing of fast and fine motor commands, and in later chapters we will see
how it is also crucial to the fluent pronunciation of language, but a 0.1 ms short-
term memory ill-serves what most scientists would call “cognition.” In compari
son with cerebral architecture, we will conclude that the cerebellar architecture
is fundamentally noncognitive. The important point here is that language is
not and cannot be learned by just any brain cells: it is, with several interesting
qualifications, really learned only by cerebral cells, and so it is to the cerebrum
that we turn our major attention.
The Cerebrum
After the skull is opened, it is the cerebrum that first presents itself to the sur
geon. On first viewing, the cerebral cortex looks like a thick placemat (or, yes,
a thin rind) about 5 mm in thickness, which has been wrinkled and stuffed
into a too-small cranium. This is the traditional view of the cerebrum, as well
as the one most familiar to the lay reader, and it is the view with which we shall
begin.
But there are more illuminating ways to look at the cerebrum. As we have
seen, Ramón y Cajal used the microscope to take a quite different view. He
put the cerebral sheet on edge and studied it from the perspective of its thick
ness. It was in this view that the cytoarchitecture of cerebral cortex was first
exposed, and this will be the second perspective from which we shall view
cerebral cortex.
58 •
HOW THE BRAIN EVOLVED LANGUAGE
A third view of the cerebrum combines the surgical and cytoarchitectural
views. This is the “planar” view of the cerebrum, and it is the view that will be
most important to our subsequent development of adaptive grammar.
The surgical view of the cerebrum:
the cerebral hemispheres
The human cerebrum is fraught with folds and bulges. These (usually larger)
fissures and lobes or (usually smaller) sulci and gyri were the basis of the earliest
anatomical attempts to describe the brain by its shape. No two brains are
wrinkled in exactly the same way, but the larger folds and bulges are common
to all human brains. Later, the finding that these common lobes often process
specific types of information gave rise to the computational metaphor of a
“modular” brain.
Like most chordate anatomies, the cerebrum (as well as the subcerebral
brain) is bilaterally organized and divided into left and right hemispheres by a
deep central sulcus. As originally suggested in figure 2.9, the right hemisphere
moves and senses the left body while the left hemisphere moves and senses
the right body, and as noted in chapter 1, language is primarily processed in
the left hemisphere. The right hemisphere is more “lateralized for” spatial tasks.
In other major respects, however, the hemispheres are essentially identical.
Figure 4.4 shows the major fissures of the brain, as well as cytoarchitec
turally distinguishable areas of cortex known as Brodmann areas. Shown are
a lateral view (an outside-in view from the side) of the left hemisphere and a
sagittal (an outside-in side view—not a dead-center, medial view) of the left
hemisphere.
Mapping motor cortex
Each cerebral hemisphere is divided by a long, vertical fissure of Rolando, the
S-shaped line running down the center of figure 4.4 (top). The frontal lobe, which
lies anterior to this central sulcus, plans actions and so is often called motor cortex.
In the 1950s through the 1970s, before the development of radiological
techniques, surgical removal was the most effective treatment for brain tumors.
Since the brain itself has no neurons which can directly sense pain, brain sur
gery only requires a local, scalp anesthetic, and after the skull has been opened,
the patient can remain conscious during the procedure. This requires great
courage on the part of the patient, but it is also a great contribution to the
safety of the procedure. With the brain exposed, the surgeon can stimulate
different regions of the brain with an electric probe while the patient reports
what he senses. Using this information, the surgeon can avoid accidentally
damaging speech areas or other especially critical areas of the brain. The com
posite of a number of such surveys revealed that many other primary motor
and sensory functions are localized in the same way that language is localized
in Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas.
THE SOCIETY OF BRAIN
•
59
Dostları ilə paylaş: |