COGNITIVE PROCESSES IN CHILDHOOD THROUGH
BODY EXPERIENCE
531
deconstructing scientifically based didactic approach, taking into account the developmental stages of the
memory and linguistic process, as well as the neural mechanisms involved in it. In these terms, and with the
aim of achieving scientific support for the benefit produced by the unstructured approach, the assumption
underlying this research was developed, assuming that such educational plans can
favor the development and
strengthening of cognitive skills, to a greater extent than the effect deriving from classic (formal) or Montessori
didactics, with reference to memory and language in pre-school children.
Experimenting Conditions
As an experimental condition of unstructured didactics, this research has employed museum didactics for
its informal learning characteristics of topics related to notions of science,
technology, art, botany, nutrition, and
sustainability, experimented through playful activities. In a document, promoted by the National Science
Council, the US Scientific Academies propose strands of science learning, i.e., six aspects of learning related to
what learners, especially those still at school, can acquire or develop from the point of view of cognitive, social,
and emotional development in museum settings. In particular, they can:
(1)
Experience excitement, interest, and motivation to learn about phenomena in the natural and physical
world;
(2) Come to generate, understand, remember, and use
concepts,
explanations, arguments, models, and facts
related to science;
(3) Manipulate, test, explore, predict, question, observe, and make sense of the natural and physical world;
(4) Reflect on science as a way of knowing; on processes, concepts, and institutions of science; and on
their own process of learning about phenomena;
(5) Participate in scientific activities and learning practices with others, using scientific language and
tools;
(6) Think about themselves as science learners and develop an identity as someone who knows about, uses,
and sometimes contributes to science.
Pellerey (2006), starting from activist theories, prefigures a didactics carried out outside of the classrooms,
but connected as a practical-behavioral application of the relational and mental skill already shaped in the child,
subdivided into many special skills that range from an intellectual comprehension of the facts (of a work, of an
object or of a machine, as of an experimental phenomenon), to
the ability to know how to practice the same things that are shown and learned in a museum (...). This
provision is the
ability to acquire skills that are themselves specialized behaviors, in a very broad sense of the term. (Pellerey, 2006, p. 14)
Braund and Reiss (2006) argue that initiatives and activities carried out by schools in informal contexts,
following museum or laboratory experiences, can complete the formal technical-scientific teaching activity and
face the vocational crisis spreading in the sector, with appropriate teaching strategies,
and a more motivating
and engaging curriculum choice. An interpretative key to their assumption is the authenticity. The structure
identified ad hoc for this phase was Explora, the Children’s Museum of Rome; in particular, for the purposes of
the research, six laboratory paths were selected, referring to
the theme of colors, digital, science, senses,
environment, and nutrition:
Dostları ilə paylaş: