In these four ways, teachers can help to create conditions that will make tasks work for acquisition. As Skehan(1998)points out, they serve to introduce new language that the learners can use while performing the task, to mobilize existing linguistic resources, to ease processing load and to push learners to interpret tasks in more demanding ways. However, it is not yet possible to ‘fine tune’learners’performance of a task through selecting specific pre-task options. At best, all that the research to date has demonstrated is the likely effects of some of the procedures referred to above. Important questions remain unanswered. For example, we do not know whether task preparation that involves an actual performance of the task is more or less effective than preparation that involves just observation. Nor is it clear to what extent linguistic priming subverts the ‘naturalness’of a task resulting in teaching of the present-practice-produce
(PPP)kind. Only in the case of strategic planning do we have some idea of how the different options affect task performance.
2.1 Task performanceoptions
The methodological options available to the teacher in the during-task phase are of two basic kinds. First, there are various options relating to how the task is to be undertaken that can be taken prior to the actual performance of the task and thus planned for by the teacher. These will be called ‘task-performance options’. Second, there are a number of ‘process options’ that involve the teacher and students in on-line decision making about how to perform the task as it is being completed.
We will consider three task performance options. The first of these options concerns whether to require the students to perform the task under time pressure. The teacher can elect to allow students to complete the task in their own time or can set a time limit. Lee(2000)strongly recommends that teachers set strict time limits. This option is important because it can influence the nature of the language students’produce. Yuan and Ellis(2003)found that giving students an unlimited time to perform a narrative task resulted in language that was both more complex and more accurate in comparison to a control group that was asked to perform the same task under time pressure. The students used the time at their disposal to monitor and reformulate their utterances. Interestingly, the opportunity to plan on-line produced a different effect from the opportunity to engage in strategic planning, which led to greater fluency and complexity of language. It seems, then, that if teachers want to emphasize accuracy in a task performance, they need to ensure that the students can complete the task in their own time. However, if they want to encourage fluency they need to set a time limit.
The second task performance option involves deciding whether to allow the students access
to the input data while they perform a task. In some tasks access to the input data is built into the design of a task(e. g. in Spot the Difference, Describe and Draw, or many information gap tasks). However, in other tasks it is optional. For example, in a story retelling/recall task the students can be permitted to keep the pictures/text or asked to put them on one side as they narrate the story. This can influence the complexity of the task, as tasks that are supported by pictures and texts are easier than tasks that are not. Joe(1998)reports a study that compared learners’acquisition of a set of target words(which they did not know prior to performing the task)in a narrative recall task under two conditions̶with and without access to the text. She found that the learners who could see the text used the target words more frequently, although the difference was evident only in verbatim use of the words not in generated use(i. e. they did not use the target words in original sentences). Joe’s study raises an important question. Does borrowing from the input data assist acquisition? The term ‘borrowing’ in this context comes from Prabhu(1987). He defines it as ‘taking over an available verbal formulation in order to express some self-initiated meaning content, instead of generating the formulation from one’s own competence(’p. 60). Prabhu distinguishes borrowing from ‘reproduction’where the decision to ‘take over’a sample of a language is not made by the learner but by some external authority (i. e. the teacher of the text book). Borrowing is compatible with task-based teaching but reproduction is not. Prabhu sees definite value in borrowing for maintaining a task-based activity and also probable value in promoting acquisition.
The third task performance option consists of introducing some surprise element into the task. Skehan and Foster(1997)illustrate this option. They asked students to complete a decision-making task that required them to decide what punishment should be given to four criminals who had committed different crimes. At the beginning of the task they were given information about each criminal and the crime he/she had committed. Half way through the task the students were given further information of a surprising nature about each criminal. For example, the initial information provided about one of the criminals was as follows:
The accused is a doctor. He gave an overdose(a very high quantity of a painkilling drug)to an
85-year-old woman because she was dying painfully of cancer. The doctor says that the woman
had asked for an overdose. The woman’s family accuse the doctor of murder.
After talking for five minutes, the students were given the following additional information: Later, it was discovered that seven other old people in the same hospital had died in a similar way, through overdoses. The doctor refuses to say if he was involved.
However, this study failed to find that introducing such a surprise had any effect on the fluency, complexity or accuracy of the learners’language. This does not mean that this option is of no pedagogic value, as requiring learners to cope with a surprise serves as an obvious way of extending the time learners spend on a task and thus increases the amount of talk. It may also help to enhance students’intrinsic interest in a task.