specific about potential outcomes given the na-
ture and etiology of the causative event or pro-
cess while leaving open the indeterminacy of
potential recovery based on time-limited obser-
vations of brain state. Because the exact fate of
an individual patient for recovery or permanent
unconsciousness is often indeterminate, the evo-
lution of brain states from coma to vegetative and
minimally conscious states to recovery without
independence to full recovery needs to be stres-
sed. The time evolution of states is often not
appreciated by surrogates who may be unduly
pessimistic or optimistic. At this juncture, it may
be prudent to caution surrogates to avoid mak-
ing a potentially premature decision and waiting
until prognostication can be informed by how
and when the patient evolves from coma.
Progression from coma to the vegetative state
does not herald additional improvement and re-
covery. This is a natural state of progression in
nearly all comatose patients, and movement into
VS is an important clinical milestone that re-
quires explanation. Surrogates need to appreci-
ate that the behaviors that are seen in VS, such
as sleep-wake cycles, blinking, roving eye move-
ments, or the startle reflex, are not purposeful
and do not indicate consciousness or awareness
of self, others, or the environment.
182
This is a
hard concept for lay people to understand. It
can be explained and emphasized that these are
automatic behaviors, much like breathing and
the maintenance of a heartbeat, controlled by
brainstem activity. Making these distinctions
is important when the patient first enters VS,
lest these behaviors be understood as evidence
of awareness or consciousness.
Discussion should emphasize that although
VS, which is as yet unmodified, may become
labeled as persistent once it has persisted for
1 month, it is not predicted to be permanent
until 3 months following anoxic injury, or 12
months when the etiology is traumatic brain
injury.
183
In the competently assessed patient,
it is clinically and ethically appropriate to assert
that patients become permanently vegetative
when they pass through these time intervals.
66
Although the 1994 Multisociety Task Force
opined that ‘‘the persistent VS is a diagnosis
and that the permanent VS is a prognosis,’’
64,65
378
Plum and Posner’s Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma
because of exceedingly rare outlier cases of
late recovery from PVS, it is reasonable to
maintain the permanent VS as a viable diagnos-
tic category if an appropriate assessment has
been made to be sure that the patient is not in
the minimally conscious state.
The minimally conscious state presents per-
haps the greatest current challenge for commu-
nication of prognosis. Although MCS is a recog-
nized plateau from which patients may regain
consistent evidence of consciousness; an aware-
ness of self, others, and their environment; and,
most critically, the ability to engage in functional
communication, the wide clinical spectrum of
MCS
184
includes some patients who will perma-
nently remain unable to communicate yet re-
tain some aspects of awareness. Because of
this complexity, ethical norms for addressing
patients in MCS are only now evolving and
likely to change as diagnostic precision improves
and therapeutic avenues open for some sub-
categories of patients. The recovery of func-
tional communication appears to represent the
principal goal of many but not all surro-
gates
70,185
involved in the care of MCS patients
(additional endpoints include self-feeding, pain
control, and emotional reactivity, among oth-
ers). Surrogates may appropriately express the
concern that waiting for further recovery from
MCS may limit later opportunities to withdraw
care so as not to abridge the patient’s prospec-
tive wishes not to remain in VS or MCS if the
condition were to be permanent.
186
Addressing
these challenges will require further engage-
ment of surrogates, physicians, and policy
makers to consider palliative goals of care for
the severely brain-injured patient.
187
Emergence from MCS is a major mile-
stone for several key reasons. First, when pa-
tients arrive at this functional level, they are
able consistently to engage others. This will
make the question of whether or not the pa-
tient is conscious indisputable and not open
to charges of familial emotionality or denial.
Second, at this more recovered state of con-
sciousness, patients more fully recapture per-
sonhood lost as the result of their injury. As the
philosopher William Winslade has observed in
an early exploration of ethical issues follow-
ing traumatic brain injury, ‘‘Being persons re-
quires having a personality, being aware of our
selves and our surroundings, and possessing
human capacities, such as memory, emotions,
and the ability to communicate and interact
with other people.’’
187a
An additional point
about emergence from MCS is that the po-
tential for recovery is open ended and unpre-
dictable. Functional capability beyond mere
emergence is an area of active research with
emerging evidence that the level of early im-
paired self-awareness may be considered as a
marker for predicting complex functional ac-
tivities later in the course of recovery from
traumatic brain injury.
188
Thus, there is a need
for ongoing assessment of capabilities and
continuing physical and occupational therapy
for patients who have managed to recover to
this state.
A final note on diagnostics is in order. Fami-
lies may want confirmatory studies to convince
them of the solidity of the clinical diagnosis,
trusting the ‘‘objectivity’’ of a scan over the anal-
ysis of the clinician. Expectations are raised by
the advent of ‘‘neuroethics’’ articles in the pop-
ular culture asserting the potential of neuroim-
aging technologies to read minds and refine
marketing techniques.
189
Because of these
trends, surrogates may invest imaging tech-
nologies with more diagnostic ability than they
currently possess and seek clearcut answers
through this visual medium. It is important to
be clear that the diagnosis and assessment of
patients with disorders of consciousness is a
clinical task informed by a competent history
and neurologic examination. Although des-
perate families may request them, as of this
writing, neuroimaging studies are only applied
in research settings and at best can be ancillary
to clinical evaluation. They must be interpreted
in light of the history and physical examination.
It is important to be transparent when discus-
sing the capabilities of current technology to
assess brain states; indicate that this is an active
area of research and caution that many of the
experimental protocols portrayed in the media
are being utilized in patients who have already
been diagnostically assessed.
190
Family Dynamics and Philosophic
Considerations
Beyond questions about the process of making
decisions and the professional obligation to ex-
change information with surrogates, it is also
important to appreciate that probabilities about
Consciousness, Mechanisms Underlying Outcomes, and Ethical Considerations
379
survival and functional status do not translate
easily into choices about human values. Sharing
prognostic probabilities is not, in itself, suffi-
cient to improve the deliberative process or to
effect outcome decisions.
Given the complexity of the decision-making
process, this is not wholly unexpected. The qual-
ity of how information was conveyed is difficult
to assess and may be as critical as what has been
conveyed. Families may be distrustful of clini-
cians and systems of care that are not designed
for longitudinal chronic care.
177
They may have
been the recipients of misinformation about
the patient’s brain state and be wary of family
meetings that they worry might try to engineer
a decision to withhold or withdraw care.
These would be formidable challenges even
if there were continuity of care and ongoing
doctor-patient/family relationships. In the
setting of shifting venues of care from the acute
hospital setting to rehabilitation and long-term
care facilities, the challenge of building trust is
formidable. To help build such relationships, it
is critical to be empathic and supportive and try
to imagine what has eloquently been described
as ‘‘the loneliness of the long-term caregiver’’
191
faced with social isolation and family members
whose injury has altered them and their rela-
tionships with those who hold them dear. These
longitudinal stresses and the dependency of
loved ones, coupled with the prognostic un-
certainties, require compassion when working
with families touched by a disorder of con-
sciousness.
Surrogates will articulate a broad range of
preferences depending on the patient’s values
and their own sense of what constitutes propor-
tionate care, from the rejection of brain death
to the decision to remove artificial nutrition and
hydration in a patient who is in a minimally
conscious state. In most cases, however, most
surrogates will struggle with the more nuanced
question of the degree of loss of self that would
make a life worth living.
This is a highly personal question. Families
may benefit by your asking them to consider
the ability to relate to others in the context of a
broader consideration about the goals of care.
This level is not reached until the patient has
recovered to the upper end of MCS or emerged
from that state. Although all may not agree with
the centrality of functional communication,
this may be a helpful goal of care when speaking
with family members. Appreciating the cen-
trality of functional communication will also
help to identify those patients who retain this
ability but need assistive devices or special
techniques to relate to others.
96
One of the most
egregious diagnostic errors that can be made in
this area of clinical practice is to mistake a
locked-in patient for one who is vegetative.
98
Locked-in patients retain the ability for func-
tional communication but need to be recog-
nized in order to mobilize emerging technol-
ogies that can correlate eye movements, or
even electrical brain activity, to the choice of
letters on a computer screen, and thereby help
locked-in patients to communicate.
94,192
Working toward the achievement of func-
tional communication can also help delineate
objectives and time frames against which this
level of function needs to be achieved lest it
simply remain an elusive hope. For example, if
it is agreed that functional communication is a
goal of care, it might be prudent to continue to
follow a patient for a year following traumatic
injury in order for a patient to have the greatest
chance of moving into the minimally conscious
state from which a capability of functional com-
munication might take root. If a patient re-
mains vegetative a year after injury, the sub-
stantially reduced chances of attaining the
communicative goal would help support a de-
cision to withdraw care.
In all of these conversations, it may be help-
ful to reach out to the hospital’s ethics com-
mittee, which will have additional expertise to
help surrogates interpret technical informa-
tion, such as patient diagnosis and prognosis,
in light of the patient’s prior wishes, prefer-
ences, and values.
REFERENCES
1. Broderick JP, Adams HP Jr, Barsan W, et al.
Guidelines for the management of spontaneous intra-
cerebral hemorrhage: a statement for healthcare pro-
fessionals from a special writing group of the Stroke
Council, American Heart Association. Stroke 30,
905–915, 1999.
2. Traumatic brain injury: Masson F, Thicoipe M,
Aye P, Mokni T, Senjean P, Schmitt V, Dessalles PH,
Cazaugade M, Labadens P. Aquitaine Group for
Severe Brain Injuries Study. Epidemiology of severe
brain injuries: a prospective population-based study.
J Trauma. 51, 481–9, 2001. Cardiopulmonary arrest:
Booth CM, Boone RH, Tomlinson G, et al. Is this
patient dead, vegetative, or severely neurologically
impaired? Assessing outcome for comatose survivors
of cardiac arrest. JAMA 291, 870–879, 2004.
380
Plum and Posner’s Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma
3. Jennett B, Teasdale G, Braakman R, et al. Prognosis
of patients with severe head injury. Neurosurgery 4,
283–289, 1979.
4. Levy DE, Bates D, Caronna JJ, et al. Prognosis in
nontraumatic coma. Ann Intern Med 94, 293–301,
1981.
5. Jennett B, Bond M. Assessment of outcome after
severe brain damage. Lancet 1, 480–484, 1975.
6. Consensus conference. Rehabilitation of persons
with traumatic brain injury. NIH Consensus Devel-
opment Panel on Rehabilitation of Persons With
Traumatic Brain Injury. JAMA 282, 974–983, 1999.
7. Jennett B. Predictors of recovery in evaluation of
patients in coma. Adv Neurol 22, 129–135, 1979.
8. Brain Trauma Foundation Management and Prog-
nosis of Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. American
Association of Neurological Surgeons, 2001.
9. Gennarelli TA, Champion HR, Copes WS, et al. Com-
parison of mortality, morbidity, and severity of 59,713
head injured patients with 114,447 patients with extra-
cranial injuries. J Trauma 37, 962–968, 1994.
10. Narayan RK, Greenberg RP, Miller JD, et al. Im-
proved confidence of outcome prediction in severe
head injury. A comparative analysis of the clinical ex-
amination, multimodality evoked potentials, CT scan-
ning, and intracranial pressure. J Neurosurg 54, 751–
762, 1981.
11. Braakman R, Gelpke GJ, Habbema JD, et al. System-
atic selection of prognostic features in patients with
severe head injury. Neurosurgery 6, 362–370, 1980.
12. Stocchetti N, Penny KI, Dearden M, et al. Intensive
care management of head-injured patients in Europe:
a survey from the European brain injury consortium.
Intensive Care Med 27, 400–406, 2001.
13. Choi SC, Narayan RK, Anderson RL, et al. En-
hanced specificity of prognosis in severe head injury.
J Neurosurg 69, 381–385, 1988.
14. Marion DW, Carlier PM. Problems with initial
Glasgow Coma Scale assessment caused by prehos-
pital treatment of patients with head injuries: results
of a national survey. J Trauma 36, 89–95, 1994.
15. Teasdale G, Knill-Jones R, van der SJ. Observer vari-
ability in assessing impaired consciousness and coma.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 41, 603–610, 1978.
16. Signorini DF, Andrews PJ, Jones PA, et al. Predict-
ing survival using simple clinical variables: a case
study in traumatic brain injury. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 66, 20–25, 1999.
17. Hukkelhoven CW, Steyerberg EW, Rampen AJ, et al.
Patient age and outcome following severe traumatic
brain injury: an analysis of 5600 patients. J Neuro-
surg 99, 666–673, 2003.
18. Jennett B, Teasdale G, Galbraith S, et al. Severe
head injuries in three countries. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 40, 291–298, 1977.
19. van Dongen KJ, Braakman R, Gelpke GJ. The prog-
nostic value of computerized tomography in comatose
head-injured patients. J Neurosurg 59, 951–957, 1983.
20. Fearnside MR, Cook RJ, McDougall P, et al. The
Westmead Head Injury Project outcome in severe head
injury. A comparative analysis of pre-hospital, clinical
and CT variables. Br J Neurosurg 7, 267–279, 1993.
21. Carlsson CA, von Essen C, Lofgren J. Factors affect-
ing the clinical course of patients with severe head
injuries. 1. Influence of biological factors. 2. Signif-
icance of posttraumatic coma. J Neurosurg 29, 242–
251, 1968.
22. Young GB. The EEG in coma. J Clin Neurophysiol
17, 473–485, 2000.
23. Young GB, Wang JT, Connolly JF. Prognostic deter-
mination in anoxic-ischemic and traumatic encepha-
lopathies. J Clin Neurophysiol 21, 379–390, 2004.
24. Logi F, Fischer C, Murri L, et al. The prognostic
value of evoked responses from primary somatosen-
sory and auditory cortex in comatose patients. Clin
Neurophysiol 114, 1615–1627, 2003.
25. Lew HL, Dikmen S, Slimp J, et al. Use of soma-
tosensory-evoked potentials and cognitive event-
related potentials in predicting outcomes of patients
with severe traumatic brain injury. Am J Phys Med
Rehabil 82, 53–61, 2003.
26. Robe PA, Dubuisson A, Bartsch S, et al. Favourable
outcome of a brain trauma patient despite bilateral
loss of cortical somatosensory evoked potential dur-
ing thiopental sedation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychi-
atry 74, 1157–1158, 2003.
27. Schwarz S, Schwab S, Aschoff A, et al. Favorable
recovery from bilateral loss of somatosensory evoked
potentials. Crit Care Med 27, 182–187, 1999.
28. Mazzini L, Zaccala M, Gareri F, et al. Long-latency
auditory-evoked potentials in severe traumatic brain
injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 82, 57–65, 2001.
29. Perrin F, Schnakers C, Schabus M, et al. Brain re-
sponse to one’s own name in vegetative state, min-
imally conscious state, and locked-in syndrome. Arch
Neurol 63, 562–569, 2006.
30. Pelinka LE, Kroepfl A, Leixnering M, et al. GFAP
versus S100B in serum after traumatic brain injury:
relationship to brain damage and outcome. J Neuro-
trauma 21, 1553–1561, 2004.
31. Shutter L, Tong KA, Holshouser BA. Proton MRS
in acute traumatic brain injury: role for glutamate/
glutamine and choline for outcome prediction.
J Neurotrauma 21, 1693–1705, 2004.
32. Levy DE, Caronna JJ, Singer BH, et al. Predicting
outcome from hypoxic-ischemic coma. JAMA 253,
1420–1426, 1985.
33. Hamel MB, Goldman L, Teno J, et al. Identification
of comatose patients at high risk for death or severe
disability. SUPPORT Investigators. Understand Prog-
noses and Preferences for Outcomes and Risks of
Treatments. JAMA 273, 1842–1848, 1995.
34. Sacco RL, VanGool R, Mohr JP, et al. Nontraumatic
coma. Glasgow coma score and coma etiology as pre-
dictors of 2-week outcome. Arch Neurol 47, 1181–
1184, 1990.
35. Sasser H. Association of Clinical Signs with Neuro-
logical Outcome After Cardiac Arrest [dissertation].
University of Pittsburg, 1999.
36. Zandbergen EG, de Haan RJ, Stoutenbeek CP, et al.
Systematic review of early prediction of poor out-
come in anoxic-ischaemic coma. Lancet 352, 1808–
1812, 1998.
37. Madl C, Kramer L, Domanovits H, et al. Improved
outcome prediction in unconscious cardiac arrest sur-
vivors with sensory evoked potentials compared with
clinical assessment. Crit Care Med 28, 721–726, 2000.
38. Rothstein TL. The role of evoked potentials in
anoxic-ischemic coma and severe brain trauma. J Clin
Neurophysiol 17, 486–497, 2000.
Consciousness, Mechanisms Underlying Outcomes, and Ethical Considerations
381
39. Rothstein TL, Thomas EM, Sumi SM. Predicting out-
come in hypoxic-ischemic coma. A prospective clin-
ical and electrophysiologic study. Electroencephalogr
Clin Neurophysiol 79, 101–107, 1991.
40. Fischer C, Luaute´ J, Adeleine P, et al. Predictive
value of sensory and cognitive evoked potentials for
awakening from coma. Neurology 63, 669–673, 2004.
41. Goh WC, Heath PD, Ellis SJ, et al. Neurological
outcome prediction in a cardiorespiratory arrest sur-
vivor. Br J Anaesth 88, 719–722, 2002.
42. Wijdicks EF, Parisi JE, Sharbrough FW. Prognostic
value of myoclonus status in comatose survivors of
cardiac arrest. Ann Neurol 35, 239–243, 1994.
43. Werhahn KJ, Brown P, Thompson PD, et al. The
clinical features and prognosis of chronic posthyp-
oxic myoclonus. Mov Disord 12, 216–220, 1997.
44. Kaplan PW. The EEG in metabolic encephalopathy
and coma. J Clin Neurophysiol 21, 307–318, 2004.
45. Golby A, McGuire D, Bayne L. Unexpected recovery
from anoxic-ischemic coma. Neurology 45, 1629–
1630, 1995.
46. Britton JW, Ghearing GR, Benarroch EE, et al. The
ictal bradycardia syndrome: localization and lateral-
ization. Epilepsia 47, 737–744, 2006.
47. Wijdicks EF, Rabinstein AA. Absolutely no hope?
Some ambiguity of futility of care in devastating
acute stroke. Crit Care Med 32, 2332–2342, 2004.
48. Pullicino PM, Alexandrov AV, Shelton JA, et al. Mass
effect and death from severe acute stroke. Neurol-
ogy 49, 1090–1095, 1997.
49. Voetsch B, DeWitt LD, Pessin MS, et al. Basilar
artery occlusive disease in the New England Medical
Center Posterior Circulation Registry. Arch Neurol
61, 496–504, 2004.
50. Rabinstein AA, Tisch SH, McClelland RL, et al.
Cause is the main predictor of outcome in patients with
pontine hemorrhage. Cerebrovasc Dis 17, 66–71,
2004.
51. Report of World Federation of Neurological Surgeons
Committee on a Universal Subarachnoid Hemorrhage
Grading Scale. J Neurosurg 68, 985–986, 1988.
52. Rosen DS, Macdonald RL. Grading of subarachnoid
hemorrhage: modification of the World Federation
of Neurosurgical Societies scale on the basis of data
for a large series of patients. Neurosurgery 54, 566–
575, 2004.
53. Schievink WI, Wijdicks EF, Piepgras DG, et al. The
Dostları ilə paylaş: |