40
CONCLUSION
In my conclusion, the analysis of emotive lexemes collected from different
online media sources made it possible to investigate various emotional expressions
used in the Internet media. The selection was carried out according to the classical
criteria of emotivity. The study results showed how quite different and quite mixed
expressive processes can construct the meaning of the message and influence the
reader. The texts of the selected online media are characterized by multimodality,
polyphony, hypertextuality, heterogeneity, and carry bifurcated denotational
meaning. It was revealed that these forms are predominantly
the most expressive
emotive process. The present study recognized and described emotive lexical units,
analyzed the different layers of information that make up the emotionality of a
media text and determine the conditions of the described lexical units’ functioning.
The pattern markers that are present in the text and influence textual variations
were also studied.
To conclude, multimodality
was also considered, which characterizes media
writing. This research shows that expression of feelings and emotions in a digital
discourse requires special linguistic and extra-linguistic means. Online media are a
place where people can express themselves freely. Hence, it can be said that media
favor self-expression. Content writers have the freedom to integrate any expressive
process. Unlike the traditional written discourse, where affective categories are
often channeled through lexicon, the digital space is associated with the presence
of linguistic and extra-linguistic layers. This study identified lexical and linguistic
means that integrate emotions into the online media texts.
Those expressive
processes are a function of the immediacy and spontaneity of media discourse.
Basic emotive vocabulary and possible extra-linguistic elements actualized in
Kazakh media discourse appeared to exploit paraverbal expressions to replace
expressive means used in face-to-face communication. Such tactics allow
conveying emotions in written debates. When it comes to digital media texts, it is
not enough to look at what has been said. Attention
should be paid to both
linguistic and non-linguistic processes. Depending on the purpose of the media
41
text, content writers who seek to influence readers can use emotive lexemes in
specific ways. Therefore, it is important to examine
emotive language from the
standpoint of linguistics and extra-linguistics. The results of this study can be used
in teaching aids and courses in philology, psychology, journalism, and semiotics.
Overall, in this study we focused on how and to which extent affective
connotations of very basic textual measures at the lexical, inter-lexical, and even
sublexical level of a poem—that can all be derived
from existing normative
databases—determine the perception of the general affective meaning of poetry in
a way that proves quantifiable beyond the specific context of a given poem, author,
or recipient. By applying an exhaustive exploratory regression analysis to a
comprehensive corpus of poems and their ratings from hundreds of readers, we
found that a significant amount of variance in discrete and dimensional affective
ratings of poetry can be accounted for solely by text-based affective measures from
different levels of processing.
In. conclusion, in all of the presented statistical models—focusing on different
aspects of the general affective meaning—variance
of each rating dimension is
significantly accounted for by affective properties of several text levels: while the
lexical one generally explains the biggest amount of variance, further significant
effects in explaining residual variance are found for the alternative sublexical and
inter-lexical text levels. Thus, our research brings together previous accounts on
specific effects of single text levels, showing how they may co-exist each in their
own right or interact to constitute the complex holistic
framework of poetry
perception. Taken together, the affective properties of text elements from all three
text levels could account for 43–70% of the variance in the perceived general
affective meaning of the here utilized poetry and still for 23–48% of the variance
in further aesthetic and onomatopoetic evaluations of the poems—a substantial
amount purely accounted for by textual elements which should not be neglected in
future affective analyses of poetry.